Thanks to Curiosity Stream for supporting PBS Digital Studios.
Hey, I’m Jabril and welcome to Crash Course AI!
Language is one of the most impressive things humans do.
It’s how I’m transferring knowledge from my brain to yours right this second!
Languages come in many shapes and sizes, they can be spoken or written, and are made up
of different components like sentences, words, and characters that vary across cultures.
For instance, English has 26 letters and Chinese has tens-of-thousands of characters.
So far, a lot of the problems we’ve been solving with AI and machine learning technologies
have involved processing images, but the most common way that most of us interact with computers
is through language.
We type questions into search engines, we talk to our smartphones to set alarms, and
sometimes we even get a little help with our Spanish homework from Google Translate.
So today, we’re going to explore the field of Natural Language Processing.
INTRO
Natural Language Processing, or NLP, mainly explores two big ideas.
First, there’s Natural Language Understanding, or how we get meaning out of combinations
of letters.
These are AI that filter your spam emails, figure out if that Amazon search for “apple”
was grocery or computer shopping, or instruct your self-driving car how to get to a friend’s
house.
And second, there’s Natural Language Generation, or how to generate language from knowledge.
These are AI that perform translations, summarize documents, or chat with you.
The key to both problems is understanding the meaning of a word, which is tricky because
words have no meaning on their own.
We assign meaning to symbols.
To make things even harder, in many cases, language can be ambiguous and the meaning
of a word depends on the context it’s used in
If I tell you to meet me at the bank, without any context, I could mean the river bank or
the place where I’m grabbing some cash.
If I say “This fridge is great!”, that’s a totally different meaning from “This fridge
was *great*, it lasted a whole week before breaking.”
So, how did we learn to attach meaning to sounds?
How do we know great [enthusiastic] means something different from great [sarcastic]?
Well, even though there’s nothing inherent in the word “cat” that tells us it’s
soft, purrs, and chases mice… when we were kids, someone probably told us “this is
a cat.”
Or a gato, māo, billee, qut.
When we’re solving a natural language processing problem, whether it’s natural language understanding
or natural language generation, we have to think about how our AI is going to learn the
meaning of words and understand our potential mistakes.
Sometimes we can compare words by looking at the letters they share.
This works well if a word has morphology.
Take the root word “swim” for example.We can modify it with rules so if someone’s
doing it right now, they’re swimming, or the person doing the action is the swimmer.
Drinking, drinker, thinking, thinker, … you get the idea.
But we can’t use morphology for all words, like how knowing that a van is a vehicle doesn’t
let us know that a vandal smashed in a car window.
Many words that are really similar, like cat and car, are completely unrelated.
And on the other hand, cat and Felidae (the word for the scientific family of cats) mean very
similar things and only share one letter!
One common way to guess that words have similar meaning is using distributional semantics,
or seeing which words appear in the same sentences a lot.
This is one of many cases where NLP relies on insights from the field of linguistics.
As the linguist John Firth once said, “You shall know a word by the company it keeps.”
But to make computers understand distributional semantics, we have to express the concept
in math.
One simple technique is to use count vectors.
A count vector is the number of times a word appears in the same article or sentence as
other common words.
If two words show up in the same sentence, they probably have pretty similar meanings.
So let’s say we asked an algorithm to compare three words, car, cat, and Felidae, using
count vectors to guess which ones have similar meaning.
We could download the beginning of the Wikipedia pages for each word to see which /other/ words
show up.
Here’s what we got:
And a lot of the top words are all the same: the, and, of, in.
These are all function words or stop words, which help define the structure of language,
and help convey precise meaning.
Like how “an apple” means any apple, but “the apple” specifies one in particular.
But, because they change the meaning of another word, they don’t have much meaning by themselves,
so we’ll remove them for now, and simplify plurals and conjugations.
Let’s try it again:
Based on this, it looks like cat and Felidae mean almost the same thing, because they both
show up with lots of the same words in their Wikipedia articles!
And neither of them mean the same thing as car.
But this is also a really simplified example.
One of the problems with count vectors is that we have to store a LOT of data.
To compare a bunch of words using counts like this, we’d need a massive list of every
word we’ve ever seen in the same sentence, and that’s unmanageable.
So, we’d like to learn a representation for words that captures all the same relationships
and similarities as count vectors but is much more compact.
In the unsupervised learning episode, we talked about how to compare images by building representations
of those images.
We needed a model that could build internal representations and that could generate predictions.
And we can do the same thing for words.
This is called an encoder-decoder model: the encoder tells us what we should think and
remember about what we just read...
and the decoder uses that thought to decide what we want to say or do.
We’re going to start with a simple version of this framework.
Let’s create a little game of fill in the blank to see what basic pieces we need to
train an unsupervised learning model.
This is a simple task called language modeling.
If I have the sentence:
I’m kinda hungry, I think I’d like some chocolate _____ .
What are the most likely words that can go in that spot?
And how might we train a model to encode the sentence and decode a guess for the blank?
In this example, I can guess the answer might be “cake” or “milk” but probably not
something like “potatoes,” because I’ve never heard of “chocolate potatoes” so
they probably don’t exist.
Definitely don’t exist.
That should not be a thing.
The group of words that can fill in that blank is an unsupervised cluster that an AI could use.
So for this sentence, our encoder might only need to focus on the word chocolate so the
decoder has a cluster of “chocolate food words” to pull from to fill in the blank.
Now let’s try a harder example:
Dianna, a friend of mine from San Diego who really loves physics, is having a birthday
party next week, so I want to find a present for ____.
When I read this sentence, my brain identifies and remembers two things: First, that we’re
talking about Dianna from 27 words ago!
And second, that my friend Dianna uses the pronoun “her.”
That means we want our encoder to build a representation that captures all these pieces
of information from the sentence, so the decoder can choose the right word for the blank.
And if we keep the sentence going:
Dianna, a friend of mine from San Diego who really loves physics, is having a birthday
party next week, so I want to find a present for her that has to do with _____ .
Now, I can remember that Dianna likes physics from earlier in the sentence.
So we’d like our encoder to remember that too, so that the decoder can use that information
to guess the answer.
So we can see how the representation the model builds really has to remember key details
of what we’ve said or heard.
And there’s a limit to how much a model can remember.
Professor Ray Mooney has famously said that we’ll “never fit the whole meaning of
a sentence into a single vector” and we still don’t know if we can.
Professor Mooney may be right, but that doesn’t mean we can’t make something useful.
So so far we’ve been using words.
But computers don’t work words quite like this.
So let’s step away from our high level view of language modeling and try to predict the
next word in a sentence anyway with a neural network.
To do this, our data will be lots of sentences we collect from things like someone speaking
or text from books.
Then, for each word in every sentence, we’ll play a game of fill-in-the-blank.
We’ll train a model to encode up to that blank and then predict the word that should
go there.
And since we have the whole sentence, we know the correct answer.
First, we need to define the encoder.
We need a model that can read in the input, which in this case is a sentence.
To do this, we’ll use a type of neural network called a Recurrent Neural Network or RNN.
RNNs have a loop in them that lets them reuse a single hidden layer, which gets updated
as the model reads one word at a time.
Slowly, the model builds up an understanding of the whole sentence, including which words
came first or last, which words are modifying other words, and a whole bunch of other grammatical
properties that are linked to meaning.
Now, we can’t just directly put words inside a network.
But we also don’t have features we can easily measure and give the model either.
Unlike images, we can’t even measure pixel values.
So we’re going to ask the model to learn the right representation for a word on its
own (this is where the unsupervised learning comes in).
To do this, we’ll start off by assigning each word a random representation -- in this
case a random list of numbers called a vector.
Next, our encoder will take in each of those representations and combine them into a single
/shared/ representation for the whole sentence.
At this point, our representation might be gibberish, but in order to train the RNN,
we need it to make predictions.
For this particular problem, we’ll consider a very simple decoder, a single layer network
that takes in the sentence representation vector, and then outputs a score for every
possible word in our vocabulary.
We can then interpret the highest scored word as our model’s prediction.
Then, we can use backpropagation to train the RNN, like we’ve done before with neural
networks in Crash Course AI.
So by training the model on which word to predict next, the model learn weights for
the encoder RNN and the decoder prediction layer.
Plus, the model changes those random representations we gave every word at the beginning.
Specifically, if two words mean something similar, the model makes their vectors more
similar.
Using the vectors to help make a plot, we can actually visualize word representations.
For example, earlier we talked about chocolate and physics, so let’s look at some word
representations that researchers at Google trained.
Near “chocolate,” we have lots of foods like cocoa and candy:
By comparison, words with similar representations to “physics” are newton and universe.
This whole process has used unsupervised learning, and it’s given us a basic way to learn some
pretty interesting linguistic representations and word clusters.
But taking in part of a sentence and predicting the next word is just the tip of the iceberg
for NLP.
If our model took in English and produced Spanish, we’d have a translation system.
Or our model could read questions and produce answers, like Siri or Alexa try to do.
Or our model could convert instructions into actions to control a household robot … Hey
John Green Bot?
Just kidding you’re your own robot.
Nobody controls you.
But the representations of words that our model learns for one kind of task might not
work for others.
Like, for example, if we trained John-Green-bot based on reading a bunch of cooking recipes,
he might learn that roses are made of icing and placed on cakes.
But he won’t learn that cake roses are different from real roses that have thorns and make
a pretty bouquet.
Acquiring, encoding, and using written or spoken knowledge to help people is a huge
and exciting task, because we use language for so many things!
Every time you type or talk to a computer, phone or other gadget, NLP is there.
Now that we understand the basics, next week we’ll dive in and build a language model
together in our second lab! See you then.
Thank you to CuriosityStream for supporting PBS Digital Studios.
CuriosityStream is a subscription streaming service that offers documentaries and non¬fiction
titles from a variety of filmmakers, including CuriosityStream originals.
For example, you can stream Dream the Future in which host Sigourney Weaver asks the question,
“What will the future look like?” as she examines how new discoveries and research
will impact our everyday lives in the year 2050.
You can learn more at curiositystream.com/crashcourse
Or click the link in the description.
Crash Course Ai is produced in association with PBS Digital Studios!
If you want to help keep Crash Course free for everyone, forever, you can join our community
on Patreon.
And if you want to learn more about how human brains process language, check out this episode
of Crash Course Psychology.